AUSTRALIAN TRANSPORT SAFETY BUREAU DOCUMENT RETRIEVAL INFORMATION Report No. ISBN ISSN Title and Subtitle
An Investigation of the Potential Interaction Between Paroxetine and Pindolol: Pharmacodynamic Assessment in Healthy Male Volunteers.
Performing Organisation
Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne, Austin & Repatriation Medical Centre,Heidelberg, Victoria, 3084 Sponsored by / Available from
Australian Transport Safety Bureau Project Officer: <First Name; Surname> GPO Box 967 CANBERRA ACT 2608
Abstract
The increasing incidence of cardiovascular disease and subsequent depressive symptoms has highlighted the potential for concomitant treatment of β-blockers and antidepressant medication. It has been noted that both types of medications are associated with decrements in psychomotor and cognitive performance. Thus, the aims of this study were to characterise the psychomotor performance characteristics in a group of healthy male volunteers who received a single oral dose of paroxetine 10 mg (an SSRI), pindolol 5 mg (a β-blocker), a placebo and a combination of paroxetine and pindolol. It was hypothesised that concurrent administration of single doses of paroxetine and pindolol will result in a significant decrease in psychomotor performance.
The study was conducted as a double blind, placebo controlled, four-way cross over study.
To assess the psychomotor effects of the administered drugs, the participants were required to perform the critical flicker fusion frequency task (CFF), choice reaction time task (CRT), Bond- Lader visual analogue scale of mood (VASM) and the digit-symbol substitution test (DSST). A significant effect between condition and time for CRT (F(3)= 7.918, p<0.001), CFF (F(3)= 2.459, p<0.05), and DSST(F(3)= 2.488, p<0.05) tests was found, which indicated that a decrement in psychomotor performance over time had occurred after concurrent administration of paroxetine and pindolol together. The explanation for the drug-drug interaction is not clear from this study, but may be due to one or more factors, such as a pharmacokinetic or pharmacodynamic interaction. Keywords
Antidepressants, paroxetine, beta-blockers, pindolol, psychomotor effects, cognitive effects
NOTES: (1) ATSB Research reports are disseminated in the interests of information exchange. (2) The views expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent those of the Commonwealth Government.
AN INVESTIGATION OF THE POTENTIAL INTERACTION BETWEEN PAROXETINE AND PINDOLOL : PHARMACODYNAMIC ASSESSMENT IN HEALTHY MALE VOLUNTEERS
Austin & Repatriation Medical Centre
Introduction
Depression is a commonly diagnosed psychological disorder. It is estimated to
affect 3-4% of the adult population at some point in their lives (Brick & Erickson,
1998). Affecting individuals of all ages, depression may be detected in patients who
display changes in their social functioning, motor behaviour, emotional states,
cognition and motivation. It is suspected that the onset of depression is due to excess
neurotransmitter receptor sites, thus leading to intrinsically lower levels of specific
monoamine neurotransmitters, namely serotonin (5-HT) and noradrenaline (NA) which
normally contribute to producing an overall sense of well being (Nevid, Rathus &
Greene, 2000). Therefore, in order to inhibit the reuptake and increase the release of
NA and 5-HT, antidepressants work by decreasing the number and sensitivity of
receptors, thus decreasing signs of depression in patients (Carvey, 1998). Several
classes of antidepressants exist, each increasing neurotransmitter levels via different
mechanisms. Selective serotonin-reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), one of the newer, more
potent and less toxic treatments, exert their effects by increasing levels of 5-HT
specifically in vitro and in vivo (Leonard, 1992; Kerr, Fairweather, Mahendran &
Cardiovascular disease has also increased in incidence in recent years. As a result
of improved medical treatments for these disorders there is an increased incidence of
survival after myocardial infarction and stroke. Beta-adrenergic receptor antagonists
(β-blockers) are commonly prescribed to treat patients after myocardial infarction and
also individuals with hypertension, arrhythmias and angina, and work to decrease the
heart rate and blood pressure in individuals (Chavey, 2000; Smith & Ball, 2000). They
are also currently used to treat other conditions such as performance anxiety, migraines
Individuals suffering from cardiovascular disease quite often present with
symptoms of depression following a myocardial infarction (Raskin, Veith, Barnes &
Gumbrecht, 1982). Between 18%-60% of patients suffering coronary artery disease
(CAD) also suffer from depression (Musselman, Evans and Nemeroff, 1998).
Furthermore symptoms of depression are both a risk factor contributing to the
development of cardiovascular disease and influence survival following myocardial
infarction (Frasure-Smith, Lesperance and Talajic, 1993). Co-occurrence of depression
and heart disease is likely to be a relatively common presentation with both disorders
requiring medication (Avorn, Everitt and Weiss, 1986). A drug-drug interaction, with
adverse consequences, in such circumstances is a distinct possibility. This may occur
by a variety of central and peripheral mechanisms.
Central Mechanisms of Interaction
The central nervous system plays a key role in coordinating sensory and motor
systems, which in turn drive psychomotor performance behaviour (Hawley, McPhee,
Quick & Smith, 1997). A decrement in functioning of psychomotor behaviours has
been noted in individuals suffering psychological disorders such as depression.
Administration of psycho-active drugs such as antidepressants can further exacerbate
impairment of psychomotor performance. Furthermore, β-blockers directly affect
central adrenergic activity and as a result are associated with central nervous system
(CNS) effects such as insomnia, depressed mood, vivid dreams, cognitive impairment
and sedation (Waal, 1967; Yudofsky, 1992).
Psychomotor performance encompasses skills and responses such as measures of
central arousal, vigilance, reaction times and motor coordination. Tests such as
critical flicker fusion (CFF) and choice reaction time (CRT) have been designed to test
for differences on these parameters as a result of sedative drug administration. Drugs
that impair psychomotor performance are said to be psychomotor toxic as they impair
the organisational processes of the CNS, which works to integrate the sensory and
motor systems (Hawley, McPhee, Quick & Smith, 1997). Antidepressants have been
shown to impair psychomotor performance in healthy volunteers. For example, the
tricyclic antidepressant (TCA) dothiepin had a sedative effect and impaired
performance on psychomotor tests involving measures of CNS arousal, sensorimotor
performance, short-term memory and psychomotor function in healthy volunteers
(Fairweather, Ashford and Hindmarch, 1996).
Antidepressants impair visual selective attention as measured by delayed
responses to peripheral and centrally placed targets in tracking tests (Weinstein,
Wilson, Bailey and Nutt, 1996; Smiley, 1987). This pattern of a decrement noted in
detection of peripheral targets is replicated in individuals suffering Parkinson’s disease.
The SSRI paroxetine may be associated with an impairment in psychomotor
performance, which is dose dependent (Kerr, Fairweather, Mahendran and Hindmarch,
1992; Hawley, McPhee, Quick and Smith, 1997; Dechant and Clissold,1991). As
measured by visual analogue scales, paroxetine may lead to feelings of drowsiness.
Furthermore, paroxetine associated feelings of fatigue, dizziness and visual
disturbances, suggest such events may be associated with impairments in psychomotor
performance (Hawley, McPhee, Quick & Smith, 1997; Preskorn, 1997).
The effects of the beta-blocker propranolol on psychomotor functioning have
been investigated (Broadhurst, 1980a; 1980b; McDevitt, 1985; Betts et al.,1985),
Decreased complex reaction times followed propanolol administration. However, this
psychomotor performance impairment was dose dependent. McDevitt (1985) also
indicated that lower doses of propranolol (40mg) have a greater effect on impairment
of psychomotor performance than higher doses (320mg), that may reduce β-blockade
effects on psychomotor functioning. Some of the main side effects elicited by beta
blockers, such as lowered blood pressure, increased sedation and interference with the
body’s adrenergic stress response, all of which decrease sensory/cognitive awareness
and reactions, may play key roles in the impairment of psychomotor behaviours
(Dimsdale, Newton & Joist, 1989; Glaister, 1981). Furthermore, propranolol has been
documented to be responsible for changes in mood and even the onset of hallucinations
Peripheral Mechanisms of Interaction
Detrimental effects of pharmacokinetic drug interactions of SSRIs as a
consequence of their effects on the metabolism of other drugs have been reported
(Richelson, 1997). SSRIs are extensively metabolised and detoxified by cytochrome
P450 (CYP) isoenzymes of the liver, however they also inhibit these enzymes. One of
the main isozymes involved in the metabolism and clearance of psychoactive drugs is
CYP2D6. Inhibition of CYP isoenzymes and CYP 2D6 in particular is the leading
cause of drug-drug interactions, and since enzyme inhibition is dose-dependent, higher
dosages result in greater inhibition. As a result of this inhibition, metabolism of other
drugs in the liver such as β- blockers (which are also metabolised by CYP2D6) would
also be affected (Preskorn, 1997). Occurrence of increased drug concentrations
indicates that potentially serious interactions may take place e.g., enhanced side effects
It has been well documented that paroxetine is a potent inhibitor of the CYP2D6
isoenzyme, and thus, of its own metabolism (Hiemke & Hartter, 2000; Jeppesen et al,
1996; Lane, 1996; Preskorn, 1997; Richelson, 1997). During chronic administration
increased blood concentrations of paroxetine may occur resulting in increased side
Specific central interactions occur when β-blockers such as pindolol are co-
prescribed with SSRI antidepressants like paroxetine. This is due to their effectiveness
in augmenting the onset of action and efficacy of SSRIs through the enhancement of
serotonergic transmission (Bordet, Thomas & Dupuis, 1998). Both drugs compete at 5-
HT receptors blocking reuptake to increase release, with studies now indicating that
pindolol has 5-HT1A receptor antagonistic properties along with its beta blocker
activity ((Haddjeri, de Montiigny & Blier, 1999; Tome, Isaac, Harte & Holland, 1997;
Zanardi et al, 1997). The readiness with which drugs cross the blood brain barrier
further indicates ways in which clinically relevant drug-drug interactions may occur.
Pindolol, along with paroxetine is lipophilic in nature, thus penetrating the blood brain
barrier very efficiently and extensively. There is convincing evidence for a central
action and the suggestion of more cognitive side effects such as sedation or lethargy
occurring than noted in non-lipophilic sedative drugs or if the drugs were taken
individually (Dimsdale, Newton & Joist, 1989; Gengo, Huntoon & McHugh, 1987).
Since both SSRIs and beta-blockers each significantly contribute to side effects
relating to psychomotor impairment and are dose dependent, co-administration may
further increase impairment of psychomotor performance due to the interactions
occurring either in the liver (metabolic) or at 5-HT receptor sites (central). Few studies
have been conducted on interactions between antidepressants and β-blockers, and the
potential effect they may pose to psychomotor performance skills, notably driving a
motor vehicle. Based on studies investigating the individual contributing effects of
antidepressants and beta-blockers on impairment in psychomotor performance, it is
suspected that adverse effects could occur if the two treatments were taken
Aim
The aims of the current investigation were to characterise the psychomotor
performance effects in a group of healthy male volunteers who received single oral
doses of paroxetine, pindolol and placebo alone and together.
Hypothesis
Based on previous studies indicating the known metabolic profile of the SSRI
antidepressant paroxetine and β-blocker pindolol, it was hypothesised that the
concurrent administration of single paroxetine doses of 10mg and single pindolol doses
of 5mg would result in a significant decrease in psychomotor performance.
Method Participants
Healthy male volunteers aged 18-45, and a weight range within 15% of the mean
weight considered ideal for height were recruited among the general public by
recruitment posters. Volunteers must have presented with normal findings from pre-
study laboratory tests and pre-study physical evaluations and have serum levels of
ALAT, ASAT, gamma-GT and creatinine under the upper normal laboratory limit. As
part of the selection procedure to determine volunteer suitability and eligibility for the
study, completion of a battery of screening questionnaires covering emotional status
and medical histories was required. These screening questionnaires comprised the
General Health Questionnaire (GHQ), Life History Questionnaire and Subject
Questionnaire. Exclusion criteria were psychiatric or medical disturbances. Once
volunteers have been deemed suitable candidates for study participation, a written
Statement of Informed Consent document was read and signed and participants were
provided with a Participant Information Sheet and given the opportunity to ask
Volunteers were required to present as drug free, agree to refrain from taking any
concomitant medications or recreational drugs throughout the testing period, and from
consuming alcohol, caffeine or tobacco within 48hrs prior to testing. Women were not
included in the study due to the possible effects of menstruation. Ethics approval was
obtained from the Drug Trials Sub-Committee and the Human Research Ethics
Committee at Austin & Repatriation Medical Centre, Heidelberg. Volunteers were not
naive to the aims and but were naive to the hypothesis of the study. At any time during
the study, for any reasons, volunteers were free to withdraw. All information, including
coded participant data collected during the course of the study was kept in a secured
locked filing cabinet and stored under a password protected computer data base.
To test pharmacodynamics, a battery of four psychomotor and cognitive
functioning tasks was administered to participants. The test battery included The Leeds
Psychomotor Performance Tester, comprising Critical Flicker Fusion (CFF) and
Choice Reaction Time (CRT) tasks. Subjective tasks included the Digit Symbol
Substitution Test (DSST) and the Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scale of Mood and
Alertness (VAS) measuring intellectual ability (VASI), physical ability (VASP) and
Pharmacodynamic Assessment: Tasks of Measurement and Materials
In order to assess psychomotor function and vigilance, all tests were specifically
sensitive to the effects of sedative drugs. If there was to be an effect exhibited at the
administered dose level, these tests were the most appropriate tests to indicate such an
outcome. The Leeds Psychomotor Tester is an apparatus used to perform the CFF and
CRT tests in healthy controls or patient samples (Hindmarch, 1994).
Results obtained in the CFF task indicate central cortical arousal and alertness. A
flashing red electroluminescent diode with varying frequency is presented to subjects.
The speed of central nervous system information processing during the increasing
frequency phase is measured via the threshold frequency (Hertz) or fusion frequency,
the point at which volunteers perceive a flashing light to become a continual
illumination (Hawley, McPhee, Quick & Smith, 1997). The volunteers are then
required to indicate their flicker threshold, the point at which the flickering is again
detected during the decreasing frequency phase. Both thresholds are constant amongst
subjects. The test ran for three minutes and the result was evaluated based on the mean
value for six alternating increasing and decreasing phases. Psychomotor
impairment was indicated via decrements within CFF threshold detection. Certain rules
were applied during the course of the CFF test to ensure standard conditions were met:
(1) since physical characteristics of the stimulus (light source intensity, light/darkness
ratio) vary from one apparatus to the next, the same apparatus was used throughout the
(2) artificial lighting conditions were kept constant in the test room;
(4) volunteers wore corrective lenses if they had refraction disorders;
(5) a constant distance of 30cm was kept between the subject’s eyes and the light
The CRT task measures the mean reaction times to stimuli via the speed of the
subject’s motor response (milliseconds) to a specific visual sensory cue. Volunteers are
required to keep the index of their dominant hand on a switch, which they then quickly
activate when a red diode is illuminated, whilst ignoring simultaneous and random
presentation of twenty-one other misleading cues, with intervals of 1 to 3 seconds
between stimuli (Hawley, McPhee, Quick & Smith, 1997). The test took about one
minute to complete. Psychomotor impairments were indicated via increased reaction
3. Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scale of Mood and Alertness (VAS)
The Visual Analogue of Scale Mood and Alertness (VAS) questionnaire is a
subjective measure of 16 analogue scales comprising three factors to assess personal
experiences/feelings of self-rated alertness (physical-VASP), self-rated calmness
(tranquillity-VAST) and self-rated contentment (intellect-VASI). This test has proven
sensitivity to a wide range of compounds (Bond & Lader, 1974).
4. Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)
The DSST is a pencil/paper test that is administered to assess the number of
correct digit/symbol substitutions the volunteer correctly allocates in a limited time 30
seconds. A legend of symbols with an allocated letter is displayed, and volunteers are
required to substitute as many symbols as possible with the correlated letter. Results
were indicative of the number of substitutions, along with the number of correct
substitutions (Wechsler, 1955). Thus, impaired individuals reflected increased errors
Drug treatments were administered according to a double blind, four-way, crossover
trial design. Volunteers were given four experimental doses of placebo, pindolol
(Visken 5mg tablets), paroxetine (Aropax 10mg tablets) and a combination of
pindolol/paroxetine. All volunteers received each of the four oral doses in a counter-
Once volunteers had satisfied selection criteria for the study, they were then
required to travel to the testing venue at the Department of Psychiatry, Austin and
Repatriation Medical Centre in Heidelberg, arriving 8am, having fasted after midnight
the previous night and having not consumed breakfast on each day of study. A light
breakfast and lunch were provided for all volunteers. Four one-day treatment sessions
were scheduled over a four- week period, with a wash-out period of 7 days between
testing days. Each session lasted approximately 9 hours, during which volunteers were
required to participate in the four psychomotor tests.
Volunteers were then familiarised with the standard psychomotor performance tasks
until a plateau in performance level was reached. The baseline data for each test was
collected at the commencement of each session, half an hour prior to drug
administration. Testing of psychomotor performance was then repeatedly assessed
following randomised administration of drug doses of either placebo, pindolol (5mg),
paroxetine (10mg) or pindolol/paroxetine at intervals 30 mins after breakfast and prior
to dosing, then 2, 4 and 8 hours after dosing. Once the final test had been completed at
the end of each day, volunteers were given taxi vouchers to return home until required
for the next session. Subjects were not permitted to drive home under any
circumstances. Following completion of 4 days of testing, a subsequent medical
examination was conducted in order to determine that volunteers had no lasting effects
Prior to acceptance and within 72 hours following completion of the current study,
haematology, blood biochemistry and urinalysis tests were performed on all
individuals. Depending on clinical indications, tests were also performed outside of
Any abnormalities detected by volunteers or the investigator defined as adverse
events, which encompass clinical symptoms/signs that may develop or worsen under
treatment, as indicated in laboratory tests or in vital signs detected during routine
evaluations, were promptly reported. An adverse event form was on hand to report
such occurrences, whereby severity was to be evaluated on a three-point scale (1=mild;
2 = moderate; 3 = severe). The causal relationship between the event and the study
drug would have been classified as excluded or not excluded.
The current study implemented a randomised, double blind, placebo controlled,
four-way crossover design. A four-factor repeated measures analysis of variance
(ANOVA) statistical testing procedure with one grouping factor (psychomotor
performance following sedative drug administration) and two repeated factors
(treatment and time of testing) was used for the main analysis. Data was subjected to
within-subject ANOVA. The completed model included psychomotor task, drug
treatment and time of testing. Prior to analyses, the Visual Analogue Scale of Mood
and Alertness (VAS) was log transformed in order to account for non-normal
distribution of scores. Statistical testing for all analyses was conducted at alpha
confidence levels of 0.05 and nomograms relating power, sample size and effect size
for a two tailed test were available. At this level of significance and with a difference
of 40% for the overall mean in psychomotor tests, it was calculated that a minimum of
sample size of 12 subjects were required to detect this difference with a power of
around 80%. Such a sample size also provided a statistically significant difference with
The independent variables were the drug dosage amounts administered to
participants during the course of testing, and the times at which tests were conducted (
-0.5, 2, 4, and 8 hours). Dependent variables were the differences between the test
values for each of the four tasks at 2, 4 and 8hrs following drug administration and
Mauchly’s tests of sphericity were used to test for normal variance. If violations
within sphericity tests were detected, a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used. Pair-
wise comparison analyses were then conducted in order to determine where the
Results
The CFF thresholds for each participant under each condition at each sample time
were obtained. The data was collated and summary statistics including the means,
standard deviations and standard errors of the means were determined. The CFF
threshold means and standard errors for each condition at each time point are depicted in
Flicker Fusion Threshold T i m e ( h o u r s ) Figure 1. Means and standard errors for CFF threshold in relation to time of drug
Inspection of the data indicates that the combination condition resulted in lower CFF
threshold scores compared to all other conditions over the first three time periods. A
repeated measures with-in subjects ANOVA showed a significant main effect of
condition F(3) = 3.731, p<0.05, and time F(3) = 21.554, p<0.001 on CFF threshold
values. The interaction observed between condition and time was also significant F(3) =
2.459, p<0.05. As there was a significant effect of the drug received on the participants’
performance, pair-wise comparisons were performed to determine when these differences
occurred. The mean CFF threshold score for the combination condition was found to be
significantly lower than all other conditions at 4 hours after drug administration. At 8
hours, pindolol had a significantly higher CFF threshold than the paroxetine condition. A
second pair-wise comparison was conducted to detect the effect of time within each of
the conditions. The CFF threshold scores observed at 8 hours were significantly greater
compared to all other time points within both the combination and placebo conditions.
Scores at 4 hours were significantly lower than scores at baseline and at 8 hours for the
CRT scores for each participant under each condition at each sample time was
obtained. Data was collated and summary statistics including the means, standard
deviations and standard errors of the means were determined. The CRT score means and
standard errors for each condition at each time point is depicted in Figure 2.
Reaction Time (secs) T i m e ( h o u r s ) Figure 2. Means and standard errors for CRT scores in relation to time of drug
Figure 2 suggests that at times 2, 3 and 4 reaction times were greater for the combination
condition than all other conditions. To determine whether these differences were
significant a repeated measures with-in subjects ANOVA was performed. A violation of
the Mauchly’s test of Sphericity for time was observed. The Greenhouse-Geisser
correction was used for this measure. A significant main effect of condition on CRT was
observed F(3) = 4.661, p<0.05, and a significant main effect of time was also observed
F(3) = 12.285, p<0.01. Furthermore, the interaction between condition and time was also
significant F(3) = 7.918, p<0.001. As there was a significant effect of condition on the
participants’ performance, pair-wise comparisons of condition across time were
conducted to determine when these differences occurred. Reaction times were
significantly longer for the CRT task for the combination condition compared to all other
conditions at 4 hours. A second pair-wise comparison was conducted to detect the effect
of time within each of the conditions. The CRT scores observed at 4 hours were
significantly greater than all other time points within the combination condition.
Reaction times recorded at 8 hours were also significantly greater to those at baseline
DSST scores for each participant under each condition at each sample time was
obtained. Data was collated and summary statistics including the means, standard
deviations and standard errors of the means were determined. The DSST score means and
standard errors for each condition at each time point is depicted in Figure 3.
Correct Substitutions T i m e ( h o u r s ) Figure 3. Means and standard errors for DSST scores in relation to time of drug
Figure 3 suggests that at all times DSST scores were the greatest for the placebo
condition compared to all other conditions. To determine any significant differences
between drug conditions a repeated measures with-in subjects ANOVA was conducted
which showed a significant main effect of condition on DSST scores F(3) = 10.081,
p<0.001, and also time on this measure F(3) = 9.610, p<0.001. The interaction observed
between condition and time was also significant F(3) = 2.488, p<0.05. As there was a
significant effect of drug on the participants’ performance, pair-wise comparisons of
condition across time were performed to determine when these differences occurred.
DSST scores were found to be significantly lower for the combination condition
compared to all other conditions at 4 hours. Pindolol was found to have resulted in
significantly lower DSST scores than paroxetine and placebo at 4 hours. An additional
difference was found between the placebo and combination conditions both at 2 and 8
hours, where the combination of drugs resulted in a significant decrease in DSST scores
compared to placebo. A second pair-wise comparison was conducted to detect the effect
of time within each of the conditions. Scores at 4 hours were significantly lower than all
other time points within the combination condition. For pindolol, scores at 4 hrs were
significantly lower than were those at baseline and at 8 hours. Scores were significantly
lower at 4 hours compared to 8 hours for the placebo group, and at 4 hours compared to
Visual Analogue Scale of Mood – Intellectual Ability (Factor 1)
Scores for the VASI factor were obtained for each participant under each
condition at each test time. Data was collated and summary statistics including the
means, standard deviations and standard errors of the means were determined. The Visual
Analogue Scale score means and standard errors for the Intellectual ability factor for each
condition at each time point is depicted in Figure 4.
Factor Score T i m e ( h o u r s ) Figure 4. Means and standard errors for Visual Analogue Scale scores (Factor 1) in
relation to time of drug administration across drug conditions.
The data suggest that the placebo condition resulted in increased Visual Analogue Scale
ratings for the Intellectual Ability Factor compared to all other conditions over all sample
times. A repeated measures with-in subjects ANOVA showed a significant main effect
of time on VASI F(3) = 19.779, p<0.001. Furthermore no significant main effect of
condition F(3) = 2.430, p>0.05, or interaction between condition and time F(3) = 0.734,
p>0.05 were observed. A pair-wise comparison was conducted to detect the effect of
time within each of the conditions. The VASI scores were found to vary significantly
across time for all drug conditions including placebo. At 4 hours scores were significantly
lower than scores at 8 hours for all drug conditions. Scores at 4 hours were significantly
lower than baseline scores for the pindolol, paroxetine and combination conditions.
Visual Analogue Scale of Mood – Physical Ability (Factor 2)
Scores for the VASP factor were obtained for each participant under each
condition at each test time. Data were collated and summary statistics including the
means, standard deviations and standard errors of the means were determined. The
Visual Analogue Scale score means and standard errors for the physical ability factor for
each condition at each time point is depicted in Figure 5.
Factor Score T i m e ( h o u r s ) Figure 5. Means and standard errors for Visual Analogue Scale scores (Factor 2) in
relation to time of drug administration across drug conditions.
The data suggest that similar Visual Analogue Scale ratings for the Physical Ability
Factor were observed for each drug condition at each test administration time. A
repeated measures with-in subjects ANOVA showed a violation of the Mauchly’s test of
Sphericity for time, condition and time by condition. A Greenhouse-Geisser correction
was used for these measures. No significant main effects of condition F(3) = 0.978,
p>0.05, time F(3) = 1.157, p>0.05, or the interaction between condition and time F(3) =
1.123, p>0.05 on VASP scores was observed. Visual Analogue Scale of Mood – Tranquillisation Effects (Factor 3)
Scores for the VAST factor were obtained for each participant under each
condition at each test time. Data was collated and summary statistics including the
means, standard deviations and standard errors of the means were determined. The Visual
Analogue Scale score mean and standard error for each condition at each time point is
Factor Score T i m e ( h o u r s ) Figure 6. Means and standard errors for Visual Analogue Scale scores (Factor 3) in
relation to time of drug administration across drug conditions.
The data suggest no real trend in Visual Analogue Scale ratings for the Tranquillisation
Effects Factor between conditions over test administration times. A repeated measures
with-in subjects ANOVA showed no significant main effect of condition on VAST F(3)
= 0.404, p>0.05. Furthermore, no significant main effect of time F(3) = 0.742, p>0.05,
or the interaction between condition and time F(3) = 1.454, p>0.05 was observed.Discussion
The hypothesis that the concurrent administration of paroxetine and pindolol
would result in a significantly greater decrement in psychomotor performance than
exclusive single doses of the two drugs was supported by the results of the CFF task, the
CRT task and the DSST. All factors of the Visual Analogue Scale failed to demonstrate
any significant differences between drug conditions at any time. The results indicated
that reaction times were significantly increased for the CRT task, CFF thresholds were
significantly reduced and more errors were made on the DSST, 4 hours after the
administration of the combination of drugs when compared to the placebo, paroxetine
and pindolol conditions. The total number of substitutions and correct substitutions made
in the DSST were also significantly decreased in the pindolol condition, 4 hours after
drug administration, compared to the placebo and paroxetine conditions.
The results of the CFF task indicate that central arousal is decreased more
significantly by the combination of drugs then by pindolol, paroxetine or placebo
administered alone. The decreased CFF threshold scores observed are indicative of
impairments in information processing, visual sensitivity and CNS arousal. The results
of the CRT task also indicate that the combination of drugs has a significantly greater
sedative effect than pindolol and paroxetine when they are administered alone.
Sensorimotor coordination and attentional monitoring abilities are significantly impaired
by the combination of drugs again indicating that such polypharmacy has pronounced
central effects. The results of the DSST indicate that the combination of paroxetine and
pindolol has a deleterious central sedative effect, with cognition being significantly
altered. Interestingly pindolol, while not affecting psychomotor performance as much as
the combination of drugs, resulted in a significantly greater decrement in DSST scores
than paroxetine and placebo. Without pharmacokinetic information, a tentative
explanation is that paroxetine potentiated the effect of pindolol when the drugs were
administered concurrently. Paroxetine may potentiate the already greater deficit due to
pindolol by inhibition of the CYP2D6 isoenzyme, effectively inhibiting the metabolism
of pindolol. Alternatively the absorption of pindolol or its total body clearance may be
affected, resulting in an increase in the plasma concentration of pindolol, and greater
psychomotor effects in the combination condition.
Although pharmacokinetic parameters were not measured some further tentative
conclusions can be made from the data. Most of the significant differences in
psychomotor performance due to drug conditions occurred 4 hours (time 3) after
administration. This is at or near the known maximum plasma concentration for pindolol
and/or paroxetine. Pindolol, which is a lipophilic molecule, is able to cross the blood
brain barrier with relative ease and speed and therefore the maximum CNS disturbances
are expected to occur proximal to the maximum plasma concentration of the drug (Rosen
An alternative explanation for the pattern of results obtained is based on
pharmacodynamic effects at central receptor sites of action. Impaired psychomotor
performance from the combination may be due to synergistic effects of pindolol and
paroxetine on central serotonin receptors. One potential site of action is the 5-HT1A
receptor. Pindolol acts as an antagonist at this auto-receptor, which results in an
accumulation of 5-HT in the synaptic cleft. Although this drug interaction has been
noted to increase the antidepressant effect of paroxetine (Tome et al., 1997; Zanardi et al.,
1997), the effect on psychomotor performance has not been established. Without blood
plasma concentration levels of either drug it is impossible to confirm or rule out any
pharmacokinetic or pharmacodynamic explanations.
For the CFF and CRT tasks, no differences were observed between the pindolol,
paroxetine and placebo conditions. The DSST however, indicated deficits in
psychomotor performance in the pindolol condition compared to the paroxetine and
placebo conditions 4 hours after drug administration. The literature surrounding the
sedative effects of antidepressants contains varying results. While significant deficits in
psychomotor performance have been observed in a number of studies using
antidepressants, the findings that the newer SSRIs have a reduced side effect profile
appear to be supported by the current study (Hawley et al., 1997). Similarly, β-blockers
have been found to consistently impair psychomotor performance but only in a dose and
temporally dependent fashion (Dimsdale et al., 1989). It appears that the psychomotor
deficits due to the administration of pindolol might be so small as only to be evident in
The results suggest that there is evidence for decreased psychomotor performance
due to the combination of paroxetine and pindolol. However, this is not evident in all
tests of psychomotor performance used. Only the objective tests, rather than the
subjective analogue scales, showed any difference between drug conditions. It may be
that the alternative tests measured different aspects of psychomotor performance and
therefore care must be taken in generalising the results from the particular tests used to all
areas of psychomotor performance. The CRT task, which measures sensory and motor
system coordination and attentional monitoring ability, is a good objective measure of
sedative effects. Likewise, the DSST is sensitive to changes in cognition due to sedative
effects, and is perhaps the most sensitive test, as differences between the combination
condition and placebo were additionally observed at 2 and 8 hours after drug
administration. Also, differences between the pindolol condition and paroxetine and
placebo were also observed using this test. The CFF test also measures sedative effects
on information processing and CNS arousal and is more contingent on visual sensitivity.
It appears that the visual system may also be significantly altered by the combination of
drugs and, lower CFF thresholds have previously been found with the lipophilic β-
blockers, metoprolol and propranolol, 2-5 hours after drug administration (Gengo et. al.,
1987; McDevitt, 1985). This is consistent with the current results and suggests that the
larger effect found from the combination of drugs might be due to poteniation of the
effects of pindolol by paroxetine. The extent to which each test measures peripheral and
central effects could also have had a bearing on the results obtained. β-blockers have
been shown to impair muscle activity and thus the results of the CRT task and the DSST
may have simply reflected a deficit in the muscle-activated response rather than CNS
effects (Broadhurst, 1980b). However, β-blockers have also been found to have
pronounced central depressant actions such as depression, disruption of sleep
architecture, hallucinations and subjective sedation (Broadhurst, 1980a; Glaister, 1981;
Kostis et al., 1990). This, together with the fact that pindolol and paroxetine are both
lipophilic, suggests that they access the CNS readily and that deficits in psychomotor
performance can be explained, at least in part, by central effects.
Although other studies have found the Bond-Lader Visual Analogue Scale to be a
good indicator of changes in subjective sedation (Kerr et al., 1992) no changes were
detected in this study. Perhaps these measures are not sensitive enough to detect changes
in sedation of the magnitude detected by the CFF, CRT and DSST tasks. Although there
was a significant increase in subjective intellectual impairment for the VAS-M
(intelligence factor) over time with each drug, this decrease was also observed in the
placebo group suggesting that the significant main effect of time was simply a reflection
of diurnal variation. The trend of greater subjective reports of intellectual impairment 4
hours after drug administration may have had more to do with boredom and diurnal
variation in lethargy and fatigue then the drugs themselves. It cannot be concluded that
the lack of significant findings for the VAS-M is due to a lack of interaction, and it is
possible that this observation was due to the acute low doses administered (as discussed
below). It could be that the DSST, CRT and CFF tests are more sensitive to the peripheral
changes that occur as a result of sedative drug administration and the subjective analogue
scales are only sensitive to CNS alterations. These tests may also prove more useful over
a longer time frame. Another consideration when using subjective ratings is the influence
of social desirability and experimenter expectations on the participant’s responses. If the
participant believed that the drugs taken were sedative in nature and the experimenter
expected them to show signs of increased drowsiness, a higher degree of subjective
Some of the inconsistencies in the present studies findings may be due to
methodological problems. The first potential problem of the experimental design was the
doses of paroxetine and pindolol used. The doses used are sub-therapeutic. Doses of 15-
20mg as opposed to the present 5mg for pindolol and 20mg as opposed to 10mg for
paroxetine are commonly used therapeutically (Dechant and Clissold, 1991; Dimsdale et
al., 1989). Doses in the normal therapeutic range were not used in the present study in
order to minimise the major side effects of nausea, vomiting and hypotension. While it
has been found that only doses of paroxetine higher than the normal therapeutic range
impair psychomotor performance (Robbe & O’Hanlon, 1995), some studies suggest that
lower doses of lipophilic β-blockers result in more significant deficits in psychomotor
performance than higher doses (McDevitt, 1985). These findings are consistent with the
results from the DSST. The dose used in the present study was an acute dose unlike the
chronic dosing that occurs therapeutically. It may be that with chronic dosing, whereby
steady state plasma concentrations are attained, greater psychomotor deficits than
reported here may occur. Alternatively, chronic dosing may result in an increase in
tolerance to the drug and a reduced side effect profile over time, as observed with the β-
blocker propranolol in the study by Broadhurst (1980b). A further methodological
problem could be the small sample size utilised resulting in a low power and an increased
probability of a Type II error, which may provide an explanation of the non-significant
findings where graphical representation has suggested an effect of drug condition on
psychomotor performance. Although strict exclusion criteria were used, this list was not
exhaustive and could not have ruled out every possible confounding factor. No
consideration of differing levels of the CYP2D6 enzyme in participants was taken which
could have significantly affected the inter-individual metabolism of the drugs,
significantly altering the results in such a small sample. A 40-fold or greater variation
between individuals in drug plasma concentrations has been demonstrated for most
psychotropic drugs (Lane, 1996). There are genetic differences in hepatic metabolism
with a small proportion (5-10%) of poor metabolisers, with reduced rates of metabolism
for substrates metabolised by the CYP2D6 isoenzyme, in the Caucasian population. If the
present sample contained one of these individuals the results may have been significantly
biased (Leonard, 1992). Poor metabolisers would have greater plasma concentrations
and would be expected to have an exaggerated pharmacologic response (Nemeroff,
The frequency of testing may also have limited the conclusions that can be drawn.
Although significant differences between drug conditions on psychomotor performance
were consistently detected at 4 hours after drug administration, the duration of
performance deficits either side of this time cannot be accurately determined from the
present studies data. As significantly greater deficits in psychomotor performance for the
combination condition compared to placebo were detected at 2, 4, and 8 hours after drug
administration, significant differences in the pindolol and paroxetine conditions may also
have occurred shortly after 2 hours and immediately prior to 8 hours. The peak plasma
concentration for paroxetine has been found to occur at approximately 5 hours after
administration, so it is possible that deficits in psychomotor performance may have been
detected at around this time if pharmacodynamic tests were performed (Dechant and
Future studies should aim to resolve some of the methodological problems
encountered in the present study in order to fully characterise the drug interaction of
pindolol and paroxetine in terms of pharmacokinetics as well as pharmacodynamics.
Studies that measure the pharmacokinetic profile of each drug, including plasma
concentration, peak concentration, time of peak concentration and bioavailability, should
be undertaken to definitively clarify when the maximum blood concentrations of the
drugs occur. Both pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic tests should be undertaken
more frequently than in the present study to further elucidate the temporal components of
the drug interaction. Studies using a larger number of subjects would minimise the
chance that a confounding factor, such as differences in hepatic metabolism, might bias
the results. Alternatively, participants could be screened genotypically or phenotypically
for CYP2D6 activity. This task could prove rather expensive and arduous because of the
large degree of variance between individuals in the population (Lane, 1996). Longer-
term studies should also be conducted whereby the drugs are chronically administered in
therapeutic doses. Despite the complexities in using females in such pharmacokinetic
studies due to their menstrual cycle, the differential alterations in sedation due to drug
administration in females is another area that warrants investigation, as there is some
evidence that plasma concentrations of SSRIs are lower in males (Preskorn, 1997). The
differential increases in plasma concentrations of paroxetine in the elderly is another
consideration with increases of 50-100% having been observed in populations of 65-75
year olds (Preskorn, 1997). This has important implications as it is this group who are
more likely to be receiving concomitant β-blocker treatment and more likely to be
adversely affected by the elevated concentration of drugs.
It appears from this study that there is a significant drug interaction when pindolol
and paroxetine are administered concurrently which results in a significant degree of
CNS depression when compared to administration of the drugs alone. This has important
implications for the population as the prescription of antidepressants and β-blockers
becomes more common. Physicians should be aware of the dangers involved in
concurrent administration of such agents. Impaired psychomotor performance may
significantly affect the execution of skilled movements involved in everyday life.
Cognitive, sedative and motor impairments and changes in visual sensitivity may affect
driving abilities and those skills involved in the operation of machinery. Different
combinations of these classes of drugs should be fully explored to establish if there is an
efficacious and safe combination of the two. If studies continue to reveal impaired
psychomotor performance due to this drug interaction alternative anti-hypertensive drugs,
with reduced side effect profiles, may need to be considered for patients already receiving
References
Avorn, J., Everitt, D.E., & Weiss, S. (1986). Increased antidepressant use in patients
prescribed β-blockers. Journal of the American Medical Association, 255,
Betts, T.A., Knight, R., Crowe, A., Blake, A., Harvey, P., & Mortiboy, D. (1985).
Effect of β-blockers on psychomotor performance in normal volunteers.
European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 28 (Suppl), 39-49.
Bond, A., & Lader, M. (1974). The use of analogue scales in rating subjective
feelings. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 47, 211-218.
Bordet, R., Thomas, P. & Dupuis, B. (1998) Effect of pindolol on the onset of action of
paroxetine in the treatment of major depression: intermediate analysis of a
double-blind placebo controlled trial. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155:
Brick, J., & Erickson, C.K. (1998). Drugs, the brain and behavior: The of abuse and dependence. New York: The Haworth Medical Press.
Broadhurst, A.D. (1980a). Comparison of effect on psychomotor performance of
single doses of propranolol and acebutolol. Current Medical Research and
Broadhurst, A.D. (1980b). The effect of propranolol on human psychomotor
performance. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 51, 176-179.
Chavey, W.E. (2000) The importance of beta blockers in the treatment of heart failure.
American Family Physician, 62: 2453-2459.
Carvey, P.M. (1998) Drug action in central nervous system, Oxford, Oxford University
Dechant, K.L. & Clissold, S.P. (1991). Paroxetine: A review. Drugs, 41, 225-253.
Dimsdale, J.E., Newton, R.P., & Joist, T. (1989). Neuropsychological side effects of
β-blockers. Archives of Internal Medicine, 149, 514-525.
Donaldson, E.J., Grant-Thomson, P., Morwood, N., O’Conner, G., & Tippett, R.
Pilot study on the value of a beta-blocking drug in initial helicopter training.
Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 51, 926-929.
Fairweather, D.B., Ashford, J., & Hindmarch, I. (1996). Effects of fluvoxamine and
dothiepin on psychomotor abilities in healthy volunteers. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behaviour, 53, 265-269.
Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., & Talajic, M. (1993). Depression following
myocardial infarction: impact on 6-month survival. Journal of the American Medical Association, 270, 1819-1825.
Gengo, F.M., Huntoon, L., & McHugh, W.B. (1987). Lipid-soluble and water-soluble β-
blockers: Comparison of central nervous system depressant effect. Archives of Internal Medicine, 147, 39-43.
Glaister, D.H. (1981). Effects of beta blockers on psychomotor performance - a
Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 52, S23-S30.
Haddjeri, N., de Montigny, C. & Blier, P. (1999) Modulation of the firing activity of rat
serotonin and noradrenaline neurons by (±) pindolol. Biological Psychiatry, 45:
Hawley, C.J., McPhee, S., Quick, S.J., & Smith, V.R.H. (1997). A review of the
psychomotor effects of paroxetine. International Clinical
Hiemke, C., & Hartter, S. (2000). Pharmacokinetics of selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 85, 11-28.
Hinshelwood, R.D. (1969) Hallucinations and propranolol. British Medical Journal, 2:
Hindmarch, I. (1994). Relevant psychometric tests for antidepressants and
anxiolytics. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 9 (Suppl), 27-33.
Jeppensen, U., Gram,L.F., Vistisen, K., Loft, S., Poulsen, H.E. and Brosen, K. (1996)
Dose dependent inhibition of CYP1A2, CYP2C19 and CYP2D6 by citalopram,
Kerr, J.S., Fairweather, D.B., Mahendran, R., & Hindmarch, I. (1992). The effects of
paroxetine, alone and in combination with alcohol on psychomotor
performance and cognitive function in the elderly. International Clinical
Kostis, J.B., Rosen, R.C., Holzer, B.C., Randolf, C., Taska, L.S., & Miller, M.H.
CNS side effects of centrally-active antihypertensive agents:a prospective,
placebo-controlled study of sleep, mood state, and cognitive and sexual function
in hypertensive males. Psychopharmacology, 102, 163-170.
Lane, R.M., (1996). Pharmacokinetic drug interaction potential of selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors. International Clinical Neuropsychology, 11 (Suppl 5),
Leonard, B.E. (1992). Fundamentals of psychopharmacology. Chichester: John Wiley
McDevitt, D.G. (1985). β-blockers and psychometric performance: studies in normal
volunteers. European Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 28, 35-38.
Musselman, D.L., Evans, D.L., & Nemeroff, C.B. (1998). The relationship of depression
and cardiovascular disease. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 580-592
Nemeroff, C.B., DeVane, L., & Pollock, B.G. (1996). Newer antidepressants and the
cytochrome P450 system. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 311-320
Nevid. J.S., Rathus, S.A. & Greene, B.(2000) Abnormal Psychology (4th Edition), New
Preskorn, S.H. (1997). Clinically relevant pharmacology of selective serotonin
reuptake inhibitors:an overview with emphasis on pharmacokinetics and effects
on oxidative drug metabolism. Clinical Pharmacokinetics, 32 (Suppl 1), 1-21.
Raskin, M., Veith., R., Barnes, R. & Gumbrecht, G.(1982) Cardiovascular and
antidepressant effects of imipramine in the treatment of secondary depression in
patients with ischemic heart disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139 : 1114-
Richelson, E. (1997) Pharmacokinetic drug interactions of new antidepressants: a review
on the effects on the metabolism of other drugs. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 72:
Robbe, H.W.J., & O’Hanlon, J.F., (1995). Acute and subchronic effects of 20 and 40
mg paroxetine on actual driving , psychomotor performance and subjective
assessments in healthy volunteers. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 5,
Rosen, R.C., & Kostis, J.B. (1985). Biobehavioural sequellae associated with
adrenergic-inhibiting antihypertensive agents: A critical review. Health
Smiley, A. (1987). Effects of minor tranquilizers and antidepressants on psychomotor
performance. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 48, 22-28.
Smith, W.H.T. & Ball, S.G (2000) ACE inhibitors in heart failure: an update. Basic Research in Cardiology, 95 : I8- I11.
Tome, M.B., Isaac, M.T., Harte, R., & Holland, C. (1997). Paroxetine and pindolol:
a randomised trial of serotonergic autoreceptor blockade in the reduction of
antidepressant latency. International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 12, 81-
Waal, H.J. (1967). Propranolol-induced depression. British Medical Journal, 2, 50.
Weschler, D (1955) A Manual for the Weschler Adult Intelligence Scale. New York,
Weinstein, A., Wilson, S., Bailey, J., & Nutt, D. (1996). Sedative antidepressants
impair visual detection mechanisms in humans. Journal of
Yudofsky, S.C. (1992). Beta-blockers and depression: the clinician’s dilemma.
Journal of the American Medical Association, 267, 1826-1827.
Zanardi, R., Artigas, F., Franchini, L., Sforzini, L., Gasperini, M., Smeraldi, E., Perez,
J. (1997). How long should pindolol be associated with paroxetine to improve
the antidepressant response? Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 446-
Summary Statistics for CFF Threshold Scores.
Pindolol Paroxetine Combination
Summary Statistics for CRT Scores.
Pindolol Paroxetine Combination Summary Statistics for DSST Scores.
Pindolol Paroxetine Combination
Summary Statistics for VAS-M Intellectual Ability Scores.
Pindolol Paroxetine Combination Summary Statistics for VAS-M Physical Ability Scores.
Pindolol Paroxetine Combination Summary Statistics for VAS-M Tranquillisation Effect Scores.
Delta Extension Resources Delta Academic Objectives: Reading Skills Leadership 2Answer key 1 1 How leadership is changing in organizations 4 In the future, a more diverse selection of people will 2 Teams are being used instead of individuals; more be in leadership roles, spanning a wider range ofwomen are leaders; cross-cultural differences are beingnationalities and races
Voci dal Sud 16 Anno IX° nr. 4 Aprile 2012 w w w . s o s e d . eu Come e perchè naque il Viagra? All’inizio fu pensato per curare l’angina pectoris e l’ipertensione, ma visti gli scarsi risultati,stava per essere accantonato, quando si pensò di valorizzare un “effetto collaterale” Yahoo Medicina Come effetto collaterale era fin troppo impor- Anzi, era ancora ma