Cognitive outcome of parietooccipital resection in children with epilepsy
Epilepsia, 51(10):2047–2057, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02651.x
Cognitive outcome of parietooccipital resection in children
*yzxSarah Lippe´, *y{Christine Bulteau, *y{Georg Dorfmuller, **Franc¸ois Audren,
{Olivier Delalande, and *y{yyIsabelle Jambaque´
*Inserm, U663, Paris, France, University Paris Descartes, Faculty of Medicine, Paris, France; APHP, Necker Hospital, Paris, France;
yFaculty of Medicine, Necker Hospital, University Paris Descartes, APHP, Paris, France; zCentre de Recherche CHU Ste, University
of Montreal, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; xDe´partement de Psychologie, Centre de Recherche en Neuropsychologie et Cognition,
Universite´ de Montre´al, Montreal, Quebec, Canada; {Unite´ de Neurochirurgie Pe´diatrique, Fondation Ophtalmologique A. de
Rothschild, Paris, France; **Service d’Ophtalmologie Pe´diatrique Fondation Ophtalmologique A. de Rothschild, Paris, France; and
yyLaboratoire de Psychologie et Neurosciences Cognitives (UMR CNRS 8189), Institut de Psychologie,
Universite´ Paris Descartes, Paris, France
ited deficits related to visual attention, object recognition,
and praxis. Nevertheless, our results suggest that brain
Purpose: We followed the neuropsychological develop-
plasticity after parietooccipital epilepsy surgery in young
ment of five children who underwent unilateral neurosur-
children allows for a schooling level of cognitive skills such
gery of the occipitoparietal lobes as a treatment for
epilepsy caused by a developmental lesion (cortical dys-
Discussion: Although recovery for visual perceptual cog-
nition was more limited than for verbal functions, long-
Methods: The follow-up period ranged from 3–7 years
term neuropsychological outcomes showed that early
surgery for epilepsy offers the possibility of optimizing
Results: Two participants had a verbal intelligence quo-
cognitive outcomes in children with posterior intractable
tient (IQ) >100 and three had a verbal IQ between 65 and
80. All five children had abnormal nonverbal IQ and exhib-
KEY WORDS: Visuospatial, Plasticity, Neurosurgery.
Neurosurgery is an accepted treatment for intractable
(Bayard & Lassonde, 2001; Helmstaedter, 2004). Scores on
partial epilepsy in pediatric and adult populations. It has
attention and working memory, episodic memory tasks, and
been proven effective, particularly for intractable temporal
naming tests have been shown to improve (Gleissner et al.,
lobe epilepsy (Olivier, 1992; Wiebe et al., 2001; JambaquØ
2005; JambaquØ et al., 2007), allowing for higher scholastic
et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2008). In children, surgery for extra-
temporal lobe epilepsy represents approximately 50% of
However, very few studies have measured the neuropsy-
procedures and has been shown successful in one-half to
chological outcomes for surgery in parietal and occipital
two-thirds of the cases (Wyllie et al., 1998; Cossu et al.,
epilepsies (Smith & Billingsley, 2001). JambaquØ et al.
2008). The most frequent cause of surgically treated extra-
(1998) reported visual apperceptive agnosia with severe
temporal intractable epilepsy is cortical dysplasia, an early
face recognition impairment, executive function deficits,
developmental malformation of the cortex (Wyllie et al.,
and autistic symptoms in a 13-year-old girl who had under-
1998; Janszky et al., 2000; Lortie et al., 2002; Lerner et al.,
gone a right occipital lobectomy for cortical dysplasia at age
7 years. Remarkably, her academic abilities in spelling,
A number of studies have reported that surgical treatment
reading, and arithmetic were higher than her functional
can improve cognitive prognosis in children with epilepsy
intelligence level, perhaps as a result of special abilitiesoften encountered in patients with autism. Cohen et al. (2004) demonstrated brain reorganization of the visual word
Accepted April 29, 2010; Early View publication June 18, 2010.
form area (VWFA) toward the right hemisphere in a young
Address correspondence to Sarah LippØ, PhD, Professeure Adjointe/
Assistant Professor, DØpartement de psychologie/Department of Psychol-
epileptic child operated at the age of 4 years for a Sturge-
ogy, UniversitØ de MontrØal/University of Montreal, C.P. 6128, Succursale
Weber syndrome in the left occipitotemporal lobe. Sinclair
Centre-Ville, MontrØal, QC, H3C 3J7 Canada. E-mail: sarah.lippe@
et al. (2005) reviewed the epileptic outcomes of nine
children with parietal resections and six with occipital
resections. Although cognitive outcome was not addressed,
ª 2010 International League Against Epilepsy
occipital lobe patients showed visual field losses. Focusing
The project was approved by the Paediatric Neuro-
on the possibility of generalized impairments, Gleissner
surgery Committee of the Fondation Ophtalmologique de
et al. (2008) assessed 15 children with parietal lobe epi-
Rothschild. Both parents and children gave informed con-
lepsy. One year after surgery, these children were seizure-
sent. We reviewed preoperative and postoperative clinical
free with postoperative improvements in attention and
investigations, which included intracranial video-electroen-
behavior, but they showed a decrease in performance intelli-
cephalography (video-EEG), magnetic resonance imaging
(MRI), and IQ findings from Wechsler or developmental
In the present study, we assessed the long-term cognitive
scales. Clinical information for each patient is detailed in
outcome—from 3–7 years postsurgery—of occipitoparietal
Table 1 and in the individual case descriptions.
epilepsy in five children who showed early onset due toTaylor-type focal cortical dysplasia. Neurosurgical resec-
tion involved the occipital lobe and temporal or parietal lobe
The follow-up period ranged from 3–7 years. All children
junctions. Neuropsychological assessment specifically tar-
underwent a neuroophthalmologic and comprehensive neu-
geted the cognitive functions of the posterior regions of the
ropsychological evaluation, designed to assess cognitive
brain. Although the occipital lobe is mainly responsible for
functions related to the posterior region of the brain. Verbal
visual perception and discrimination, the parietal lobe is
and nonverbal intellectual functions were evaluated preop-
important for visuospatial orientation, attention, saccades,
eratively and postoperatively on Wechsler or developmental
numerical cognition, grapheme–phoneme transcoding, esti-
scales (Table 2). Postoperatively, more extensive neuropsy-
mating time and quantities, and praxis (Leff et al., 2001;
chological and ophthalmologic evaluations were performed.
Hubbard et al., 2005; Pouthas et al., 2005; Cavanna &
The neuroophthalmologic evaluation included visual acuity
Trimble, 2006). Occipitotemporal areas are involved in
and the Goldmann visual field examination. We used stan-
color, object, and face recognition; reading; and other func-
dardized tests to assess the following five subdomains of
tions (Dehaene et al., 2001; Casarotto et al., 2008). In both
nonverbal cognition: basic visual discrimination, visual
children and adults, the impact of a lesion in the occipital
attention, visual recognition, spatial perception, and praxis.
and parietal lobes can result in visual agnosia, neglect, vi-
In addition, we evaluated working memory, executive func-
suospatial disorientation, alexia, sensorial extinction, and
tions, and academic skills such as reading and numerical
Gerstmann syndrome (Laurent-Vannier et al., 2003). Given
the early maturation of the posterior areas of the brain, we
Screening tests from the Visual Object and Space Percep-
predicted reduced recovery of visuospatial abilities as com-
tion (VOSP) battery (Warrington & James, 1991) were used
pared to language development. Within the posterior
to assess the basic visual discrimination level of each child.
regions, we also hypothesized that lesion type and location
We measured visual attention with cancellation tasks: the
(right- or left-sided resection) would differentially impact
NEPSY Visual Attention Task (Korkman et al., 1998) and
cognitive function, given that the right and left hemispheres
the Bell test from the Batterie d’Øvaluation de la nØgligence
show distinct preferences for Gestalt versus analytical pro-
unilatØrale du Geren (BEN) (Rousseaux et al., 2001). Sub-
cessing (Van Kleeck & Kosslyn, 1989). We posited that
tests for eye and head deviation, extinction, and line bisec-
neurodevelopmental lesions, such as cortical dysplasia and
tion were used from the Batterie to assess symptoms of
early onset epilepsy, would entail reduced recovery of
neglect. Visual recognition was tested through naming and
visuoperceptual cognition compared to verbal function.
designation of objects (JambaquØ & Dellatolas, 2000), dis-
Because the literature reports of long-term follow-up in
crimination of overlapping and embedded figures (Ghent,
children with postoperative occipitoparietal epilepsy are
1956), and recognition of degraded figures (Gestalt closure)
scarce, our neuropsychological case descriptions from 3–
(Kaufman & Kaufman, 1983). For facial recognition, we
7 years postsurgery focused on visuospatial abilities and
used the short form of the Benton Facial Recognition Test
scholastic skills such as reading and arithmetic.
We evaluated spatial perception using the NEPSY
Arrows subtest (Korkman et al., 1998) and the Benton
Judgement of Line Orientation Test (JLOT) (Benton,
1983a; Benton et al., 1983b), with children’s norms
Eleven children with medically refractory epilepsy
(Lindgren & Benton, 1980). To measure visuoconstructive
underwent surgical resection of the posterior region of the
abilities and praxis, we used the Rey Complex Figure Test
brain at the Fondation Ophtalmologique Rothschild de Paris
(Rey, 1959) and NEPSY Hand Movements and Visuomotor
(France) between 1999 and 2007 (Figs S1 and S2). Children
were selected based on age at surgery (£7 years of age), cur-
Reading was assessed by subtests from two batteries: the
rent age (between 8 and 12 years), and postoperative status
Test de l'Alouette (Lefavrais, 1967) and the Outil de DØpi-
(seizure-free or greatly improved). Five children fulfilled
stage des Dyslexies (ODEDYS) (Neurocognition, 2002).
the inclusion criteria and agreed to participate in the study.
In both cases, accuracy and speed were measured.
Epilepsia, 51(10):2047–2057, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02651.x
Outcome of Posterior Resection in Children
ND, not done; VIQ, Verbal Intelligence Quotient; PIQ, Performance Intelligence Quotient.
aGlobal Developmental Quotient (French Gesell scale adaptation).
Numerical cognition was evaluated by the Zareki-R bat-
a right hemisphere lesion. In four children, between 20
tery of scores (2006), which includes assessment of visual
and more than 100 seizures were recorded during the
counting, mental counting, number writing, number read-
period of intracranial video-EEG monitoring, whereas
ing, oral mental calculation, visual quantity estimation, and
one patient, Patient 2, presented with only two seizures.
The preoperative IQ scores revealed a range from mild
Visual span and working memory were tested with the
deficit (Patient 1) to mental delay (Patients 3 and 5), and
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Fourth Edition
autistic features in one patient (Patient 4). All children
(WISC-IV) Integrated Spatial Span subtest. Auditory atten-
exhibited poor visual perception and recognition in every-
tion and working memory were tested further by compari-
day life. The posterior part of the occipitotemporal gyrus
son to visual tasks, using the NEPSY Auditory Attention
was included in the occipital and posterior parietal lobe
and the WISC-IV Integrated Digit Span tests. Executive
resections (Figs S1 and S2) in all participants but one
function was assessed through the NEPSY Tower subtest,
and behavior was evaluated on the Child Behaviour CheckList (CBCL) questionnaire (Achenbach, 1991) filled out by
Postoperatively, all children improved their intellectual
Standardized neuropsychological test scores were classi-
abilities, but with significant differences between verbal
fied into two categories, according to the mean and standard
and nonverbal components (see Table 2 and detailed
deviation (SD) of each test: 1.5 SD below the mean was
description). Although all nonverbal IQs were abnormal
qualified as a ‘‘deficit,’’ whereas 1.0 SD above or below the
and three children had a verbal IQ below 80, two had verbal
mean were qualified as ‘‘average.’’ With some exceptions, a
IQs above 100. Parental rating of behavior (on the CBCL
pathological threshold was used: for the Benton Facial Rec-
questionnaire) was average for four of the five children.
ognition Test, this was more than two points from the mean
Nevertheless, behavioral difficulties were observed during
(Benton), and for the embedded figures task, two omissions
our evaluations and are reported in the case descriptions.
(Ghent, 1956). For the scoring of the BEN subtests (Bell
All participants displayed normal visual discrimination
test, Eye and Head Deviation, Extinction and Line Bisec-
(VOSP). However, all performed at least 1.5 SD below the
tion), young adult norms were used as a guideline (Rous-
mean on object naming (DEN-48), visuomotor praxis (hand
seaux et al., 2001). Finally, T scores above 60 on the CBCL
movements), and regular word reading (ODEDYS-time)
questionnaire (Achenbach, 1991) were rated pathological.
tasks (Table 3). Four of the five children performed at least1.5 SD below the mean on visual attention tests (Bell andNEPSY), visual perceptual integration skills (degraded
forms Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children [K-ABC]),
embedded figure discrimination (Ghent), face recognition
All five children experienced seizure onset in the early
(Benton), visuospatial orientation (Benton JLOT and
months of life, characterized by various types of clinical
NEPSY Arrows), visuoconstructive praxis (Rey figure),
ocular manifestations. The seizures were stereotypic par-
visuomotor precision (NEPSY), and irregular word reading
tial seizures beginning with a conjugate gaze deviation or
and pseudo-word reading time (ODEDYS) (Table 1).
a strabismus followed by ocular clonus; one child (Patient
On the other hand, only Patient 4 did not show normal
2) presented infantile spasms, which were well controlled
auditory attention and planning abilities (NEPSY Auditory
by Vigabatrin. MRI lesions were visible in the occipital
Attention and NEPSY Tower, SD < )1.5). The two chil-
lobe in all children (Patients 1–5) as well as in the parietal
dren with normal verbal IQ (Patients 1 and 2) performed
lobe in three (Patients 1–3) and in the temporoparietal
above the mean on the planning task. Numerical abilities
junction in one (Patient 4). Only one child, Patient 1, had
such as number reading and writing, counting, and verbal
Epilepsia, 51(10):2047–2057, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02651.x
Outcome of Posterior Resection in Children
showed visual attention deficits. On the line-bisection task,
Table 3. Patients showing deficit or average score
she exhibited left visual neglect, and was also impaired on
object recognition, with poor visual perceptual integration
performance on object naming and Gestalt closure tests.
The patient was highly impaired when face recognition
involved lighting and angle variation between the target and
responder, contrasting her perfect performance on identical
Spatial perception was markedly impaired (Benton JLOT
and the NEPSY Arrows task). On the copy of the Rey figure,
she could not easily estimate shape dimensions or distance
between elements. She showed also dyspraxia, with marked
difficulty in imitating hand movement positions, and poor
This patient was significantly slow at reading single
words and text, and made more mistakes reading pseudo-
words and regular words than irregular words. On the other
hand, she was not significantly impaired on numerical cog-
nition. She could count items, count backwards mentally,
read and write numbers, do oral mental arithmetic (addition,
subtraction, multiplication), and estimate visually and ver-
Patient 2. Seizures were characterized by left eye and
head deviations and ocular clonus, associated with infantile
spasms and psychomotor regression one month after the ini-
tial presentation. The preoperative neurologic examination
showed mild axial hypotonia, poor socialization skills with
impaired visual contact, if any, and an inability to catch
objects. The right eye was affected with strabismus. After
neurosurgery and introduction of low-dose carbamazepine,
the patient showed amazing development of language andinteraction skills.
and visual estimation were within average range for four ofthe five children (exception, Patient 3).
At 8 years old, this boy showed right inferior quadranop-
Case descriptions of participants with normal
sia, hypermetropia, astigmatism, and strabismus. On hemi-
neglect tasks, he showed extinction signs in the left ear,
Patient 1. Experienced seizures at birth with ictal and
whereas the line-bisection test was significantly skewed to
interictal right occipital EEG activity. Seizures were charac-
the right. On the Bell cancellation task, he omitted more
terized by upward left elevations of the eye globes and
stimuli in the left hemispace than the right. Furthermore, he
ocular clonus. After the second neurosurgery, at age
exhibited visual inattention with disorganized visual search-
5 years, the child became seizure-free without need for
ing abilities. Object recognition was impaired and even
medication, although upward left ocular movements were
more so when the forms were degraded or embedded. On
still present at random instances. The visual field test
the Benton Facial Recognition Test, he was slow and made
showed left hemianopsia. This girl was 9.7 years old at the
mistakes on the matching of identical front-view photo-
time of the neuropsychological evaluation.
The patient exhibited left hemianopsia with upward left
Spatial perception was relatively preserved but the child
was greatly impaired on visuomotor precision, calligraphy,
explored her visual field mainly with her right eye and
and the imitation of hand positions.
Reading assessment showed that word reading was
slower than expected, but error rates were normal or better
hypermetropia, and right hemianopsia. On cancellation
than average. Furthermore, text reading performance was
tasks, the patient omitted a large number of items on the left
within normal range. The child showed normal numerical
and right fields but was still able to explore the entire visual
field. She showed extinction phenomena only in the tactilemodality. This girl is the only child in our sample displaying
Case descriptions of participants with abnormal
aural–verbal as well as visual attention deficits. Visuospa-
tial memory and verbal working memory were both
Patient 3. This patient was 19 months old at epilepsy
impaired. Object recognition was altered (naming and des-
onset. The EEG showed ictal and interictal left occipitopa-
ignation), mainly for degraded forms and embedded figures.
rietal abnormal activity. At 5 years of age, this left-handed
Object-naming errors were mainly semantic (e.g., gripper
boy had permanent deviation of the right eye and tended not
named as screwdriver). On the other hand, she could per-
to sufficiently use the right side of his body. He exhibited
fectly recognize front-view photographs of the face but was
language delay but was characterized by evident visual
very much impaired when skew and lighting conditions
agnosia and dyspraxia. At 8 years 8 months of age, the
patient still showed a lack of behavioral control.
The patient showed impaired visuospatial abilities on the
Ophthalmologic examination showed a right eye devia-
Benton JLOT and the NEPSY Arrows task. The copy of the
tion, which was irregularly present. Assessed through can-
Rey figure was segmented and she could not estimate the
cellation tasks, he exhibited visual attention difficulties with
dimensions of shapes or the distances between elements.
a similar number of omissions on each side of the hemi-
The child exhibited marked impairment during hand move-
space. In addition, he showed significant symptoms of
ment and visuomotor precision tasks.
apperceptive agnosia. He could recognize identical figures,but object recognition, especially for degraded and embed-
ded figures, was altered. He made mistakes on both semanti-
This girl showed the greatest reading deficits of all the
colexical (e.g., stairs for ladder) and visual (e.g., caterpillar
participants, as she was significantly slow at reading single
for snail) levels. On the Benton Facial Recognition Test, the
words and text. Furthermore, the number of mistakes was
child’s performance was also highly impaired.
similar whether she was reading regular words, irregularwords, or pseudo-words. She made visual, phonologic, and
regularization mistakes. On the other hand, she was not sig-
Visuospatial orientation abilities as well as constructive
nificantly impaired on numerical cognition. She could count
items, read and write numbers, and estimate verbally con-textualized quantities. However, the visual quantities esti-
mation task was riddled with attention difficulties; in fact,
Reading abilities were relatively preserved for text. The
this patient showed hyperactivity and lack of behavioral
boy showed only a 1-year lag in reading speed for regular,
irregular, and pseudo-words. Unlike other children in our
Patient 5. Preoperatively, the EEG revealed ictal and inte-
sample, he was significantly impaired on numerical cogni-
rictal left parietooccipital abnormal activity. Seizures
tion, although, especially number reading and writing.
occurred with left upward eye movements and ocular clo-
Many mistakes were visuospatial, for example, reading 305
nus. At the age of 2 years, the child had mild psychomotor
as 135, but other errors involved a misrepresentation of
delay and interacted poorly with his mother. Behavioral dif-
numerical knowledge (reading 138 as one thirteen eight;
ficulties related to food, with signs of anorexia. Two neuro-
reading 6,485 as 74,895). Visual and verbal contextual esti-
surgeries were needed to obtain freedom from seizures. The
mation abilities, however, were normal.
boy was 10 years and 8 months old at the time of the neuro-
Patient 4. The first neurologic examination found psycho-
motor delay and abnormal eye movements. Seizuresoccurred with upward right elevations of the eye globes and
body, and the EEG showed ictal and interictal left occipital
The ophthalmologic examination showed right hemia-
activity. Postoperative MRI revealed blurring of the white–
nopsia, but the patient compensated well. Although he
gray matter junction over the left posterior temporal lobe,
showed right visual extinction when stimulated bilaterally,
which extended anteriorly to the resection. The patient was
he had a lesser level of visual attention deficit than the other
11 years of age at the time of the neuropsychological evalu-
children in the sample. Visuospatial working memory was
within average. However, this boy could not name objects
Epilepsia, 51(10):2047–2057, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02651.x
Outcome of Posterior Resection in Children
properly despite preserved visual perceptual integration
neglect syndrome, an attention deficit disorder that causes
abilities. He could recognize front-view photographs of
patients to demonstrate pathologic bias for ipsilesional
faces, but was impaired when lighting and skew varied
items, neglecting the contralateral ones (Mattingley et al.,
between the target and the responder.
1997). The one child with a right-sided lesion (Patient 1)was the most impaired on spatial attention and showed signs
of neglect. Our observations are consistent with those of
The patient showed well-preserved visuospatial orienta-
recent studies reporting that the right temporoparietal junc-
tion abilities but poor spatial construction abilities. He also
tion, part of which had been resected in Patient 1, is related
was affected with dyspraxia, poor imitation of hand posi-
to the network involved in reorienting behaviors following
attentional capture by other stimuli (Corbetta & Shulman,2002; Mitchell, 2008). In addition, neglect has recently been
described in children with left and right parietal acquired
This patient was one of the most impaired in reading
lesions (Laurent-Vannier et al., 2003). In our sample, only
skills, being significantly slow at single words and text.
Patient 1 with a right occipitoparietal lesion showed neglect
Errors were made while reading pseudo-words, regular
of the contralateral hemispace. With regard to developmen-
words, and irregular words, making visual mistakes 28% of
tal lesions, our results do not confirm the previously prevail-
the time. On the other hand, numerical cognition tasks were
ing hypothesis of more frequent spatial neglect of the right
easily performed. Assessed by NEPSY tests, auditory atten-
hemispace in very young children (Laurent-Vannier et al.,
tion was within average, but digit span and verbal working
Most children were impaired on degraded object recogni-
tion or object naming. Object recognition is known toinvolve the lateral occipital and the fusiform cortex bilater-
ally (Eger et al., 2004, 2007; Pegna et al., 2004) and to be
All children experienced seizure onset before 2 years of
modulated by top-down processes by the frontoparietal net-
age. After surgery, three children were seizure-free and off
work. Children were unable to recognize or name an object
medication, whereas the remaining two showed infrequent
without contextual cues, which suggests that recovered
seizure reoccurrence and received one antiepileptic drug.
brain processes related to seizure freedom and/or brain plas-
Four of five children attained normal schooling level. Ver-
ticity failed to completely compensate for the unilateral pos-
bal IQ was improved by up to 35 points after the surgery. As
terior lesions. Interestingly, the one child who could
in children with Sturge-Weber syndrome with drug-resistant
accurately identify degraded objects had the lateral occipital
epilepsy, the control of epilepsy and reduction of pharmaco-
cortex (LOC) preserved out of his left lesion (Patient 5),
therapy may have prevented severe cognitive impairments
whereas the analogous LOC-sparing surgery in a right hemi-
(Sujansky & Conradi, 1995). Three to seven years postoper-
sphere lesion still impaired the child on the same task
atively, all children showed significant discrepancies
(Patient 1). The right hemisphere is typically associated
between verbal and nonverbal IQs. Although two of five
with spatial transformation of a mental Gestalt, a process
children displayed normal verbal IQ, all were moderately
likely to be involved in degraded object recognition tasks
impaired on nonverbal IQ. Our observations are in concor-
(Warrington & James, 1991; Humphreys et al., 1992). Our
dance with the significant postsurgery difference between
observations, therefore, confirm the bilateral LOC role in
verbal and nonverbal IQ reported in adults with posterior
resections, regardless of side of surgery or visual field loss
Facial recognition, tested using the Benton Facial Recog-
nition Test, was impaired in four of the five children. The
Our neuropsychological evaluation suggests that a history
task is separated into two parts. Only the first part does not
of early severe epilepsy with a focal cortical dysplasia fol-
require visuospatial skills, as it shows front-view photo-
lowed by surgical resection will have an impact on the cor-
graphs; the second part shows the angles and lighting in a
responding visual, spatial, and academic cognitive abilities.
different view. The lesions and cognitive difficulties experi-
All five children showed cognitive deficits related to visual
enced by the subject are reflected in corresponding deficits
attention, object recognition, praxis, and reading speed.
on the first or second part of the task. For example, the onlychild showing normal performance on orientation tasks (a
patient with a small lateral resection in the left hemisphere),
The posterior parietal lobe is considered to have a special
made mistakes on the first section but very few on the sec-
role in spatial functions (Nachev & Husain, 2006). Indeed,
ond. In contrast, the most impaired child on orientation
visual spatial attention was impaired in all five children.
tasks, with a median right hemisphere resection, showed a
Subregions such as the temporoparietal junctions are
perfect performance on the first part, but a worse perfor-
involved in the spatial attention network (Mesulam, 1981;
mance on the second part of the test. Other reports on facial
Vallar et al., 1993). They are strongly associated with
perception have revealed limited postlesional plasticity in
the pediatric population resulting from both right and left
preserved left fusiform gyrus. Cohen et al. (2004) reported
lesions (Mancini et al., 1994; de Schonen et al., 2005). Our
hemispheric shift for reading but not for language in a child
study supports the heterogeneity of the mechanisms related
of preschool age at the time of left occipital resection. In
to developmental prosopagnosia (Duchaine & Nakayama,
Patient 4, the large left hemispheric dysplasia, the early
resection (6 years of age), and early seizure freedom andreduction of pharmacotherapy could have resulted in brain
reorganization toward the right hemisphere allowing for a
All children showed dyspraxia, ranging from movement
certain level of reading. In all cases, it is possible that visual
imitations to constructions. Movement imitation is thought
field loss may have altered the saccade movements, causing
to involve a complex network in the frontal and parietal
a reduction in reading speed. Inefficient fixation during
regions (Iacoboni et al., 1999; Shmuelof & Zohary, 2007).
reading has been described in hemianoptic patients
Authors have proposed that the anterior intraparietal area is
6 months after the acquired lesion (McDonald, 2006).
involved in a kinesthetic copy of the movements to be used
Although it should be considered for the two least impaired
during action execution (Iacoboni et al., 1999), whereas the
children, who mainly showed alteration in reading speed
inferior parietal lobule is associated with body schema
instead of accuracy, this alternative explanation cannot hold
knowledge (Berlucchi & Aglioti, 1997). Although the ante-
true in the most impaired children because both accuracy
rior intraparietal area was not altered in most of our chil-
dren, the epilepsy and the resections, with the inferior
Although the impact of developmental lesions and neuro-
parietal lobe partially resected in most children, must have
surgery did not create alexia in any of the children, it did
produced a disconnect in the neural network that prevented
impair all of them on word reading speed. Parietal regions
them from performing accurate gesture imitations. As
of the brain are involved in spatial attention processes, ori-
expected, visuomotor precision and complex figure copying
enting and decoding letters and words, and consequently in
were impaired, being associated with a number of brain
early learning of lexical functions. Epelbaum et al. (2008)
areas in the frontal, parietal, and occipital lobes bilaterally,
showed that disconnecting the occipital cortex from the
such as the cuneus (Tzagarakis et al., 2009), which had been
VWFA (by altering the inferior longitudinal fasciculus) was
resected in most children in our sample.
sufficient to provoke reading deficits. Therefore, it wouldseem that up to 7 years postsurgery, a right or left posteriorlesion of the extent seen in our subjects prevents the child
from performing at normal reading capacity at school age,
but does not prevent the individual from learning how to
All children were impaired with regard to reading speed.
Reading is a complex task involving an extended network. Word recognition has recently been strongly associated with
the ventral stream, where a functional specialization for
Numerical cognition refers to processes such as subitiz-
recurrent spelling patterns was found in the left occipitotem-
ing (estimation for quantities lower than four), estimating,
poral sulcus, named the visual word form area (VWFA)
counting, calculating, and comparing quantities or numbers.
(Cohen et al., 2003; Marinkovic et al., 2003; McCandliss
Studies have demonstrated the role of a frontoparietal net-
et al., 2003; Binder et al., 2006; Gaillard et al., 2006). The
work for numerical cognition (Roland & Friberg, 1985;
dorsal stream is thought to be involved in word recognition,
Dehaene et al., 1996). Within this network, the intraparietal
when serial reading ensues from degraded special properties
sulcus regions, bilaterally, are specifically involved in most
of words or spatial rearrangement of letters (Gitelman et al.,
numerical cognition processes. In children, comparison
2005; Vinckier et al., 2006; Cohen et al., 2008). Graph-
tasks of Arabic numerals and color nonnumerical symbols
eme–phoneme conversion and semantic processing of
induced bilateral intraparietal sulcus activity in normal chil-
words involve left temporal and frontal regions of the brain
dren but not in children showing developmental dyscalculia
(Simos et al., 2002; Mechelli et al., 2005).
(Mussolin et al., 2010). Only one child in our sample was
The two most impaired children had lesions spread over
impaired on numerical cognition, the one in whom the
the left ventral stream (Patients 3 and 5). Patient 3 had an
largest part of the superior parietal lobule was resected
extensive left dysplasia and Patient 5 had a resection of the
(Patient 3). The child showed deficits in number reading,
left fusiform gyrus (including the VWFA) at 7 years of age.
number writing, and dot counting. He did not show deficits
Indeed, plasticity may have been limited in Patient 5 by vir-
on visual estimation, whereas the one child demonstrating
tue of age and the fact that he had acquired a level of reading
difficulties on the estimation tasks had an occipitotemporal
expertise before the surgery. The two children with the least
lesion (Patient 4). A plausible explanation is that the estima-
impaired reading speed had very distinct lesions. Patient 4
tion task was modulated by spatial working memory
had extensive left dysplasia combined with a resected fusi-
(Culham & Kanwisher, 2001), which was altered in the
form gyrus, whereas Patient 2 had a small neurosurgery with
latter (Patient 4) but not the former (Patient 3).
Epilepsia, 51(10):2047–2057, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02651.x
Outcome of Posterior Resection in Children
Patient 1, the only child with a right occipitoparietalresection, was the most impaired as concerns visual atten-
tion, visuospatial orientation, simultagnosia, and visuospa-
Our sample allowed us to directly compare two chil-
tial construction abilities. In contrast, Patient 5, with a
dren with very early epilepsy onset and lesions of the
very similar resection of the left occipitoparietal region,
occipital-temporoparietal region, one in the left hemi-
showed the least visual attention and visuospatial working
sphere (Patient 5) and one in the right (Patient 1). Both
memory difficulties, but was the most impaired on read-
children were impaired as to reading, but showed a double
ing. Finally, the only patient in whom the resection
dissociation in two aspects. First, although Patient 5 was
involved the left occipital-temporoparietal region (Patient
significantly impaired on irregular word reading, Patient 1
3), including the superior parietal lobe, was the only one
showed normal performance. The lexicosemantic path
to show numerical cognition impairments and had rela-
necessary for irregular word reading is thought to involve
the anterior part of the inferior frontal gyrus and of the
From our small series, we can only convey that a number
fusiform gyrus (Mechelli et al., 2005), which had been
of variables including dysplasia size, resection size, and
resected in Patient 5 but not in Patient 1. Lesioned in the
location are important in predicting verbal and nonverbal
left hemisphere, Patient 5 was greatly impaired in lexical
cognitive outcomes. However, given that verbal IQ
knowledge. In contrast, Patient 1 was able to directly read
improvements occurred after surgery, we can ascertain a
with the left hemisphere, where lexical knowledge is well
beneficial role for seizure freedom and antiepileptic drug
developed. Second, although Patient 1 showed significant
reduction in verbal brain processing. The mechanisms
pseudo-word reading errors, Patient 5 had normal perfor-
responsible for the visuospatial outcomes remain unclear.
mance. Words are first processed within each hemisphere
Whereas performance IQ did not significantly change after
in the V4 area and then transferred through the corpus
the surgery, the level of competence of each child is still
callosum to the left hemisphere, in the VWFA, around the
striking when one considers the lesion sizes in some of the
temporooccipital sulcus (Cohen et al., 2000). Pseudo-word
children. Possible recovery factors in visuospatial tasks may
reading may further involve the dorsal premotor cortex
include distinct cognitive strategies, brain reorganization,
(Mechelli et al., 2005). Our results suggest that pseudo-
and normalized brain activity resulting from seizure free-
word reading may in fact involve other networks,
including the right hemisphere. Both the right and left
One limitation of this study includes the fact that the more
hemisphere fusiform gyri process newly presented words
extensive neuropsychological evaluation was conducted
(Dehaene et al., 2001), and the posterior fusiform area has
only postoperatively. This limitation comes from the young
been shown to correlate with the frontal region involved
age of the patients included in this study. For future work,
in pseudo-word reading (dorsal premotor cortex) (Mechelli
more comprehensive preoperative assessment could help in
et al., 2005). We posit a right hemisphere contribution in
better characterizing their improvements or worsening after
the early detection of pseudo-words that could mimic
serial reading processes known to activate biparietal
In conclusion, early surgical intervention in children with
regions (Vinckier et al., 2006; Cohen et al., 2008).
posterior intractable epilepsies offers the possibility of opti-
In summary, our sample was too small to quantify cog-
mizing cognitive outcome and quality of life. We believe
nitive outcome with respect to dysplastic tissue, localiza-
that, in future, more attention should be paid to neuropsy-
tion, resection size, time of surgery, seizure freedom, and
chological assessment of these patients to better understand
pharmacotherapy reduction. However, the visuospatial
their visual perceptive deficiencies and provide better clini-
impairment profile of our sample can be related to the
neuroanatomic framework. We observed that the one child(Patient 2) showing the highest nonverbal IQ score wasalso the one with the smallest dysplastic tissue as visual-
ized on MRI, had the least amount of resection, and was
This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health
operated on at the youngest age. Indeed, this patient
Research (Lippe S) and UnitØ de Neurochirurgie PØdiatrique, Fondation
showed a very good outcome considering the classic long-
Ophtalmologique A. de Rothschild, Paris, France Inserm UMR663. Epi-
term prognosis of infantile spasms (JambaquØ et al.,
lepsies de l’enfant et plasticitØ cØrØbrale, UniversitØ Paris Descartes,Paris, France.
1993). Furthermore, the child showing the worst overallresults (Patient 4), including deficits in oral–verbal atten-tion, also showed the most extended dysplasia, involving
the left temporal lobe. The other four patients may also
We confirm that we have read the journal’s position on issues
show residual nonepileptogenic dysplastic tissue not obvi-
involved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent
ous on MRI. In these cases, the dysplastic tissue may or
with those guidelines. None of the authors has any conflict of interest to
may not be cognitively functional (Janszky et al., 2000).
Ghent L. (1956) Perception of overlapping and embedded figures by chil-
dren of different ages. Am J Psychol 69:575–587.
Gitelman DR, Nobre AC, Sonty S, Parrish TB, Mesulam MM. (2005) Lan-
Achenbach T. (1991) Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18
guage network specializations: an analysis with parallel task designs
and 1991 profile. Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont,
and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 26:975–985.
Gleissner U, Sassen R, Schramm J, Elger CE, Helmstaedter C. (2005)
Bayard S, Lassonde M. (2001) Cognitive, sensory and motor adjustment to
Greater functional recovery after temporal lobe epilepsy surgery in chil-
hemispherectomy. In JambaquØ I, Lassonde M, Dulac O (Eds) Neuro-
psychology of childhood epilepsy. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publish-
Gleissner U, Kuczaty S, Clusmann H, Elger CE, Helmstaedter C. (2008)
Neuropsychological results in pediatric patients with epilepsy surgery
Benton A. (1983a) Facial recognition. Les Editions du Centre de Psycholo-
in the parietal cortex. Epilepsia 49:700–704.
Helmstaedter C. (2004) Neuropsychological aspects of epilepsy surgery.
Benton A, Hamsher K, Varney NR, Spreen O. (1983b) Judgment line orien-
Epilepsy Behav 5(suppl 1):S45–S55.
tation form H. Contribution to neuropsychological assessment. Oxford
Hubbard EM, Piazza M, Pinel P, Dehaene S. (2005) Interactions between
number and space in parietal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:435–448.
Berlucchi G, Aglioti S. (1997) The body in the brain: neural bases of corpo-
Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ, Quinlan PT, Price CJ, Donnelly N. (1992)
real awareness. Trends Neurosci 20:560–564.
Parallel pattern processing and visual agnosia. Can J Psychol 46:377–
Binder JR, Medler DA, Westbury CF, Liebenthal E, Buchanan L. (2006)
Tuning of the human left fusiform gyrus to sublexical orthographic
Iacoboni M, Woods RP, Brass M, Bekkering H, Mazziotta JC, Rizzolatti G.
(1999) Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286:2526–
Casarotto S, Bianchi AM, Ricciardi E, Gentili C, Vanello N, Guazzelli M,
Pietrini P, Chiarenza GA, Cerutti S. (2008) Spatiotemporal dynamics of
JambaquØ I, Chiron C, Dulac O, Raynaud C, Syrota P. (1993) Visual inat-
single-letter reading: a combined ERP-FMRI study. Arch Ital Biol
tention in West syndrome: a neuropsychological and neurofunctional
imaging study. Epilepsia 34:692–700.
Cavanna AE, Trimble MR. (2006) The precuneus: a review of its functional
JambaquØ I, Mottron L, Ponsot G, Chiron C. (1998) Autism and visual
anatomy and behavioural correlates. Brain 129:564–583.
agnosia in a child with right occipital lobectomy. J Neurol Neurosurg
Cohen L, Dehaene S, Naccache L, Lehericy S, Dehaene-Lambertz G,
Henaff MA, Michel F. (2000) The visual word form area: spatial and
JambaquØ I, Dellatolas G. (2000) Epreuves de fluence verbale et de dØnom-
temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal
ination chez l’enfant d’âge scolaire. ANAE 56:13–16.
subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain 123(Pt 2):291–307.
JambaquØ I, Dellatolas G, Fohlen M, Bulteau C, Watier L, Dorfmuller G,
Cohen L, Martinaud O, Lemer C, Lehericy S, Samson Y, Obadia M, Sla-
Chiron C, Delalande O. (2007) Memory functions following surgery for
chevsky A, Dehaene S. (2003) Visual word recognition in the left and
temporal lobe epilepsy in children. Neuropsychologia 45:2850–2862.
right hemispheres: anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral
Janszky J, Jokeit H, Schulz R, Hoppe M, Ebner A. (2000) EEG predicts sur-
alexias. Cereb Cortex 13:1313–1333.
gical outcome in lesional frontal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 54:1470–
Cohen L, Lehericy S, Henry C, Bourgeois M, Larroque C, Sainte-Rose C,
Dehaene S, Hertz-Pannier L. (2004) Learning to read without a left
Kaufman AS, Kaufman NL. (1983) Kaufman Assessment Battery for Chil-
occipital lobe: right-hemispheric shift of visual word form area. Ann
dren (K-ABC). American Guidance Service[Adaptation franÅaise: Bat-
terie pour l’examen psychologique de l’enfant.] 1993. Editions du CPA,
Cohen L, Dehaene S, Vinckier F, Jobert A, Montavont A. (2008) Reading
normal and degraded words: contribution of the dorsal and ventral
Kim SK, Wang KC, Hwang YS, Kim KJ, Chae JH, Kim IO, Cho BK.
visual pathways. Neuroimage 40:353–366.
(2008) Epilepsy surgery in children: outcomes and complications. J
Corbetta M, Shulman GL. (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-dri-
ven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3:201–215.
Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S. (1998) NEPSY: a developmental neuropsy-
Cossu M, Lo Russo G, Francione S, Mai R, Nobili L, Sartori I, Tassi L, Cit-
chological assessment. The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio,
terio A, Colombo N, Bramerio M, Galli C, Castana L, Cardinale F.
(2008) Epilepsy surgery in children: results and predictors of outcome
Laurent-Vannier A, Pradat-Diehl P, Chevignard M, Abada G, De Agostini
M. (2003) Spatial and motor neglect in children. Neurology 60:202–
Culham JC, Kanwisher NG. (2001) Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in
human parietal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11:157–163.
Lefavrais P. (1967) Test de l'Alouette. Les Øditions du Centre de Psycholo-
Dehaene S, Tzourio N, Frak V, Raynaud L, Cohen L, Mehler J, Mazoyer B.
(1996) Cerebral activations during number multiplication and compari-
Leff AP, Scott SK, Rothwell JC, Wise RJ. (2001) The planning and guiding
son: a PET study. Neuropsychologia 34:1097–1106.
of reading saccades: a repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation
Dehaene S, Naccache L, Cohen L, Bihan DL, Mangin JF, Poline JB, Riviere
D. (2001) Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious rep-
Lerner JT, Salamon N, Hauptman JS, Velasco TR, Hemb M, Wu JY, San-
etition priming. Nat Neurosci 4:752–758.
kar R, Donald Shields W, Engel J Jr, Fried I, Cepeda C, Andre VM,
de Schonen S, Mancini J, Camps R, Maes E, Laurent A. (2005) Early brain
Levine MS, Miyata H, Yong WH, Vinters HV, Mathern GW. (2009)
lesions and face-processing development. Dev Psychobiol 46:184–208.
Assessment and surgical outcomes for mild type I and severe type II
Duchaine BC, Nakayama K. (2006) Developmental prosopagnosia: a win-
cortical dysplasia: a critical review and the UCLA experience. Epilep-
dow to content-specific face processing. Curr Opin Neurobiol 16:166–
Lindgren SD, Benton AL. (1980) Developmental patterns of visuospatial
Eger E, Henson RN, Driver J, Dolan RJ. (2004) BOLD repetition decreases
judgment. J Pediatr Psychol 5:217–225.
in object-responsive ventral visual areas depend on spatial attention. J
Lortie A, Plouin P, Chiron C, Delalande O, Dulac O. (2002) Characteristics
of epilepsy in focal cortical dysplasia in infancy. Epilepsy Res 51:133–
Eger E, Henson RN, Driver J, Dolan RJ. (2007) Mechanisms of top-down
facilitation in perception of visual objects studied by FMRI. Cereb Cor-
Luerding R, Boesebeck F, Ebner A. (2004) Cognitive changes after epi-
lepsy surgery in the posterior cortex. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
Epelbaum S, Pinel P, Gaillard R, Delmaire C, Perrin M, Dupont S, Dehaene
S, Cohen L. (2008) Pure alexia as a disconnection syndrome: new diffu-
Mancini J, de Schonen S, Deruelle C, Massoulier A. (1994) Face recogni-
sion imaging evidence for an old concept. Cortex 44:962–974.
tion in children with early right or left brain damage. Dev Med Child
Gaillard R, Naccache L, Pinel P, Clemenceau S, Volle E, Hasboun D,
Dupont S, Baulac M, Dehaene S, Adam C, Cohen L. (2006) Direct
Marinkovic K, Dhond RP, Dale AM, Glessner M, Carr V, Halgren E.
intracranial, FMRI, and lesion evidence for the causal role of left infero-
(2003) Spatiotemporal dynamics of modality-specific and supramodal
temporal cortex in reading. Neuron 50:191–204.
word processing. Neuron 38:487–497.
Epilepsia, 51(10):2047–2057, 2010doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2010.02651.x
Outcome of Posterior Resection in Children
Mattingley JB, Driver J, Beschin N, Robertson IH. (1997) Attentional com-
Sinclair DB, Wheatley M, Snyder T, Gross D, Ahmed N. (2005) Posterior
petition between modalities: extinction between touch and vision after
resection for childhood epilepsy. Pediatr Neurol 32:257–263.
right hemisphere damage. Neuropsychologia 35:867–880.
Smith M, Billingsley RL. (2001) Neuropsychology of parieto-occipital
McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. (2003) The visual word form area:
epilepsy. In JambaquØ I, Lassonde M, Dulac O (Eds) Neuropsychology
expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn Sci 7:293–
of childhood epilepsy. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers, New
McDonald SA. (2006) Parafoveal preview benefit in reading is only
Sujansky E, Conradi S. (1995) Outcome of Sturge-Weber syndrome in 52
obtained from the saccade goal. Vision Res 46:4416–4424.
Mechelli A, Crinion JT, Long S, Friston KJ, Lambon Ralph MA,
Trauner DA. (2003) Hemispatial neglect in young children with early uni-
Patterson K, McClelland JL, Price CJ. (2005) Dissociating reading
lateral brain damage. Dev Med Child Neurol 45:160–166.
processes on the basis of neuronal interactions. J Cogn Neurosci
Tzagarakis C, Jerde TA, Lewis SM, Ugurbil K, Georgopoulos AP. (2009)
Cerebral cortical mechanisms of copying geometrical shapes: a multidi-
Mesulam MM. (1981) A cortical network for directed attention and unilat-
mensional scaling analysis of fMRI patterns of activation. Exp Brain
eral neglect. Ann Neurol 10:309–325.
Mitchell JP. (2008) Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selec-
Vallar G, Antonucci G, Guariglia C, Pizzamiglio L. (1993) Deficits of posi-
tive for theory-of-mind. Cereb Cortex 18:262–271.
tion sense, unilateral neglect and optokinetic stimulation. Neuropsycho-
Mussolin C, De Volder A, Grandin C, Schlogel X, Nassogne MC, Noel MP.
(2010) Neural correlates of symbolic number comparison in develop-
Van Kleeck MH, Kosslyn SM. (1989) Gestalt laws of perceptual organiza-
mental dyscalculia. J Cogn Neurosci 22:860–874.
tion in an embedded figures task: evidence for hemispheric specializa-
Nachev P, Husain M. (2006) Disorders of visual attention and the posterior
tion. Neuropsychologia 27:1179–1186.
parietal cortex. Cortex 42:766–773.
Vinckier F, Naccache L, Papeix C, Forget J, Hahn-Barma V, Dehaene S,
Neurocognition LC-saLdPe. (2002) ODESYS Outil de DØpistage de la
Cohen L. (2006) ‘‘What’’ and ‘‘where’’ in word reading: ventral coding
Dyslexie. UIFM de Grenoble, Grenoble.
of written words revealed by parietal atrophy. J Cogn Neurosci
Olivier A. (1992) Temporal resections in the surgical treatment of epilepsy.
Warrington EK, James M. (1991) A new test of object decision: 2D silhou-
Pegna AJ, Khateb A, Michel CM, Landis T. (2004) Visual recognition of
ettes featuring a minimal view. Cortex 27:370–383.
faces, objects, and words using degraded stimuli: where and when it
Wiebe S, Blume WT, Girvin JP, Eliasziw M. (2001) A randomized, con-
occurs. Hum Brain Mapp 22:300–311.
trolled trial of surgery for temporal-lobe epilepsy. N Engl J Med
Pouthas V, George N, Poline JB, Pfeuty M, Vandemoorteele PF, Hugue-
ville L, Ferrandez AM, Lehericy S, Lebihan D, Renault B. (2005) Neu-
Wyllie E, Comair YG, Kotagal P, Bulacio J, Bingaman W, Ruggieri P.
ral network involved in time perception: an fMRI study comparing long
(1998) Seizure outcome after epilepsy surgery in children and adoles-
and short interval estimation. Hum Brain Mapp 25:433–441.
Rey A. (1959) Test de copie d'une figure complexe. Les Øditions du Centre
Roland PE, Friberg L. (1985) Localization of cortical areas activated by
thinking. J Neurophysiol 53:1219–1243.
Rousseaux M, Beis JM, Pradat-Diehl P, Martin Y, Bartolomeo P,
Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
Bernati T, Chokron SM, Leclercq M, Louis-Dreyfus A, Marchal
F, Perennou DC, Prairial C, Rode G, Samuel C, Sieroff E, WiartL, Azouvi P. (2001) PrØsentation d’une batterie de dØpistage de la
Figure S1. Postsurgery MRI of Patient 2.
nØgligence spatiale. Normes et effets de l’âge, du niveau d’Øducation,
Figure S2. Postsurgery MRI of Patient 5.
du sexe, de la main et de la latØralitØ. Rev Neurol (Paris) 157:1385–
Please note: Wiley-Blackwell is not responsible for the
Shmuelof L, Zohary E. (2007) Watching others’ actions: mirror representa-
content or functionality of any supporting information sup-
tions in the parietal cortex. Neuroscientist 13:667–672.
plied by the authors. Any queries (other than missing mate-
Simos PG, Breier JI, Fletcher JM, Foorman BR, Castillo EM, Papanicolaou
rial) should be directed to the corresponding author for the
AC. (2002) Brain mechanisms for reading words and pseudowords: anintegrated approach. Cereb Cortex 12:297–305.
Veterinary antibiotics in pig feces Relationship between the longevous population and trace element in the School of Environment, Beijing Normal University, Beijing soils of Xiayi County, China The intense livestock farming in China engenders a Institute of Geographical Sciences and Natural Resources significant volume of manures with no other solution for farmers than land
ACTA SESION ORDINARIA Nº 04/ 2013 CONCEJO MUNICIPAL Viernes 01.02.2013 En Yumbel, en la Sala de Sesiones del Municipio, a las 09:00 horas del día Viernes 01 de Febrero del año 2013, se da inicio a la Sesión Ordinaria Nº 04 del presente año, del Concejo Municipal, presidida por el Alcalde Camilo Enrique Cabezas Vega, y con asistencia de los Concejales señores/as, Gacitúa, Jiménez,